A statistical learning perspective on children's learning about graphotactic and morphological regularities in spelling.

拼写 心理学 透视图(图形) 语言学 认知科学 认知心理学 人工智能 计算机科学 哲学
作者
S. Hélène Deacon,Nicole J. Conrad,Sébastien Pacton
出处
期刊:Canadian Psychology [American Psychological Association]
卷期号:49 (2): 118-124 被引量:119
标识
DOI:10.1037/0708-5591.49.2.118
摘要

We put forward our view on how children learn to spell through a review of recent research on English and French children's spelling development. We examine children's learning of graphotactic conventions (legal combinations of letters), followed by a more in-depth treatment of how children learn the place of morphemes (smallest units of meaning in language) in spelling. We contrast findings from recent research with those of traditional models that suggest that children use both graphotactic and morphological information relatively late in their spelling careers and that the end-point of development, particularly for morphological conventions, lies in rule-based performance. Instead, it seems that quite young children's spellings are influenced by both graphotactic and morphological patterns and that writers do not rely (at least exclusively) on rules. We consider the possibility that children might use statistical learning to gain knowledge of both graphotactic and morphological features of the orthography. Keywords: morphology, spelling acquisition, orthographic learning, spelling development The English and French orthographies adhere to several types of regularities: specifically, phonological, graphotactic, and morphological. In this article, we put forward our view on how children's spellings are influenced by these consistencies, with a special focus on learning of graphotactic and morphological regularities. Our approach draws on the statistical learning perspective, in which it is important to consider the regularities in the (written) input on which children could rely. Accordingly, we begin by considering the kind of information represented in the English and French orthographies. French and English, like most alphabetic orthographies, rely in part on the regular connections between letters and sounds to generate word spellings. This is the case for the words cat and non (no). And yet this phonological basis does not explain the variability with which sounds are represented across different words. For example, the /eI/ sound can be spelled with -ay, -ey, -eigh, -a, and -ea at the ends of words in English and with -ez, -ai, -e, -ef, -ee, and -aie at the end of words in French. A second source of information useful to spelling comes from graphotactic regularities about the legal combinations of letters.' In French, words cannot end in a consonant doublet on its own (e.g., pomme, but not *pomm_ for the word apple in French), but they can in English. In English it is extremely unusual for words to start with consonant doublets (e.g., full, but not *fful), and such patterns never occur in French. These graphotactic patterns determine, in part, the specific letters that make up the spellings of words in both English and French. A third level of consistency in spelling comes from the smallest units of meaning in language, or morphemes. The use of morphological information determines the choice between several plausible representations of a given sound. The decision between the many different spellings of/e/ (as in bed, head, and said) is simple for some words when morphology is considered (e.g., ea in health driven by the root heal). Similarly, the sound /et/ has many different spellings in French (e.g., -aite, -ete or -ete), but a word ending in a diminutive suffix is always spelled -ette (as in fillette, little girl). Morphology even sometimes overrides the phonological basis of spelling. English regular past tense verbs end in the letters -ed, despite the pronunciation of these letters as either /t/, /d/ or /Id/ (as in walked, warned, and spotted). Similarly, in French the letters -ai are used for the sound 3 in faiseur (/fazr/, producer) to preserve the spelling of the root (faire /fer/, to do), despite the fact that e is the typical spelling of this sound. In the above examples, morphology could be useful, but success in spelling could also be achieved through word-specific memorization. …
最长约 10秒,即可获得该文献文件

科研通智能强力驱动
Strongly Powered by AbleSci AI
更新
PDF的下载单位、IP信息已删除 (2025-6-4)

科研通是完全免费的文献互助平台,具备全网最快的应助速度,最高的求助完成率。 对每一个文献求助,科研通都将尽心尽力,给求助人一个满意的交代。
实时播报
田様应助firefox采纳,获得10
1秒前
大方万仇完成签到 ,获得积分10
1秒前
happy8le完成签到,获得积分10
4秒前
季冬十五完成签到,获得积分10
4秒前
自然归尘完成签到 ,获得积分10
5秒前
量子星尘发布了新的文献求助10
5秒前
6秒前
隐逸者完成签到,获得积分10
6秒前
6秒前
lemon完成签到,获得积分10
8秒前
5433完成签到 ,获得积分10
10秒前
Tinweng完成签到 ,获得积分10
12秒前
科研通AI2S应助transition采纳,获得10
13秒前
13秒前
13秒前
ylyn发布了新的文献求助10
16秒前
18秒前
FCL完成签到,获得积分10
20秒前
蔺天宇完成签到,获得积分10
21秒前
carly完成签到 ,获得积分10
22秒前
123完成签到 ,获得积分10
23秒前
WeiMooo完成签到 ,获得积分10
24秒前
pyf不懂科研完成签到 ,获得积分10
25秒前
翁醉山完成签到,获得积分10
25秒前
YY完成签到 ,获得积分10
26秒前
量子星尘发布了新的文献求助10
26秒前
28秒前
Haonan完成签到,获得积分10
28秒前
菠萝吹雪完成签到,获得积分10
30秒前
30秒前
科研通AI2S应助transition采纳,获得10
33秒前
夜信完成签到,获得积分10
33秒前
万邦德完成签到,获得积分10
33秒前
34秒前
34秒前
biosep完成签到,获得积分10
34秒前
砳熠完成签到 ,获得积分10
35秒前
hbj完成签到,获得积分10
35秒前
友好灵松完成签到,获得积分10
35秒前
海上森林的一只猫完成签到 ,获得积分10
35秒前
高分求助中
International Code of Nomenclature for algae, fungi, and plants (Madrid Code) (Regnum Vegetabile) 1500
Functional High Entropy Alloys and Compounds 1000
Building Quantum Computers 1000
Molecular Cloning: A Laboratory Manual (Fourth Edition) 500
Social Epistemology: The Niches for Knowledge and Ignorance 500
优秀运动员运动寿命的人文社会学因素研究 500
Principles of Plasma Discharges and Materials Processing,3rd Edition 500
热门求助领域 (近24小时)
化学 材料科学 医学 生物 工程类 有机化学 生物化学 物理 内科学 纳米技术 计算机科学 化学工程 复合材料 遗传学 基因 物理化学 催化作用 冶金 细胞生物学 免疫学
热门帖子
关注 科研通微信公众号,转发送积分 4236571
求助须知:如何正确求助?哪些是违规求助? 3770240
关于积分的说明 11841061
捐赠科研通 3426808
什么是DOI,文献DOI怎么找? 1880732
邀请新用户注册赠送积分活动 933251
科研通“疑难数据库(出版商)”最低求助积分说明 840160